Copyright
©The Author(s) 2021.
World J Psychiatr. Dec 19, 2021; 11(12): 1366-1386
Published online Dec 19, 2021. doi: 10.5498/wjp.v11.i12.1366
Published online Dec 19, 2021. doi: 10.5498/wjp.v11.i12.1366
Ref. | Setting | Aim | Statistical methods | Limitations |
Soderstrom et al[50], 2002 | NeuropsychiatricClinic in Sweden | To study the personality characteristics of adults with AS | One sample t-test | Small sample size |
Anckarsäter et al[47], 2006 | Neuropsychiatric Clinic in Sweden | To describe PD in relations to ADHD and ASD symptoms | One sample t - test | Non-specific symptoms may be overselected |
Ketelaars et al[43], 2008 | Center of Expertise for Autism in Netherlands | To explore difference between patients with mild ASD and patients without ASD in term of AQ scores and psychiatric comorbidity | Χ2 test | Small sample size |
Rydén and Bejerot[40], 2008 | Psychiatric setting (tertiary unit) in Sweden | To characterize psychiatric patients with ASD in regard to demographical factors, psychiatric comorbidity and personality traits and compare the ASD group with a psychiatric control group; to compare differences of personality traits between females and males in the ASD group. | Fisher exact test; t-test; Kruskal-Wallis test | Not ADOS/ADI-R for assessing ASD; A naturalistic study |
Hofvander et al[14], 2009 | Neuropsychiatric Hospital in France NeuropsychiatricClinic in Sweden | To describe the clinical presentation and psychosocial outcome of a group of normal intelligence adults with ASD | Χ2 test | Lack of comparison group; Two studies sites; Prevalence of comorbid psychiatric conditions may be overestimated |
Sizoo et al[49], 2009 | Two diagnostic centers specialized for adult patients with developmental disorders in Netherlands | To test whether adults with ASD or ADHD have distinct personality profiles, to assess how personality profiles in these groups differed by SUD status | One sample t-test | The clinically based diagnostic procedures; The absence of a psychiatric control group; All participants were diagnosed in adulthood |
Geurts and Jansen[44], 2011 | Tertiary psychiatric unit from diagnosing ASD in Netherlands | To draw the pathway to a diagnosis for adults referred to ASD assessment | Mann-Whitney U tests; Kruskal-Wallis tests; Χ2 test | Retrospective chart study; Not standardized clinical interviews for assessing axis I and axis II diagnosis |
Kanai et al[59], 2011 | University Hospital in Japan | To examine the clinical characteristics of adults with AS | Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient | Small sample size |
Kanai et al[67], 2011 | University Hospital in Japan | To examine the clinical characteristics of adults with AS | Mann-Whitney U test | Small sample size |
Lugnegård et al[38], 2012 | Neuropsychiatric clinics in Sweden | To explore the presence of PD in young adults with AS | Χ2 test | Small sample size |
Schriber et al[55], 2014 | Local recruitment by physicians, psychologists, speech and language pathologists, occupational therapists, advocacy groups, regional centers, ASD support groups in United States | To compare self-reports of Big Five personality traits in adults with ASD to those of typically developing adults. | Independent sample t-test | Small sample size |
Hesselmark et al[62], 2015 | Tertiary psychiatric unit for diagnosing ASD; a community based facility for ASD; a website for ASD | To test validity and reliability of self-report data using the NEO-PI-R in adults with ASD | Independent sample t-test | Small sample size |
Strunz et al[26], 2015 | Department of Psychiatry at a University Hospital in Germany | To identify personality traits in adults with ASD and to differentiate them from patients with NPD, BPD and NCC | MANOVA | Selection bias (BPD and NPD were inpatients, while ASD were outpatients) |
Helles et al[52], 2016 | Neuropsychiatric Centre in Sweden | To examine temperament and character in males who were diagnosed with AS in childhood and followed prospectively over almost two decades | t-test; Kruskal-Wallis H testDunn’s post hoc test | Only men with AS |
Schwartzman et al[56], 2016 | On line recruitment United States | To assess and compare personality traits of adults with and without elevated ASD traits using; the Five Factor Model of personality | Independent sample t-test | Online administration of self-report questionnaires; Sample was not representative of adult population with ASD |
Vuijk et al[51], 2018 | Expertise Centre for Autism in Netherland | To investigated temperament and character dimensions of men with ASD by individual case matching to a comparison group. | t-test | Only men with ASD |
Ozonoff et al[65], 2005 | University Child and Adolescent specialized clinic in United States | To explore personality and psychopathology in adult with ASD | Independent sample t-test | Small sample size |
López-Pérez et al[95], 2017 | Four different mental health institutions in Spain | To examine use of different interpersonal ER strategies in BPD and AS compared to normative control participants | ANOVA | Self-reports of interpersonal ER; ToM was not assessed |
Dudas et al[92], 2017 | CARD, online responders to a website | To compare ASC, BPD, and comorbid patients in terms of autistic traits, empathy, and systemizing | ANOVA | Diagnosis was based on self-report of patients |
Murphy[100], 2006 | High security psychiatric care in UK | To compare the ToM performance of three forensic patient groups (AS, Schizophrenia and PD patients) | Kruskal-Wallis H test | No control for the potential influence of medication on cognitive functioning |
Stanfield et al[87], 2017 | Clinical and support services in Scotland; Nonpsychotic people who had previously participated in the EHRS of schizophrenia | To compare Social Cognition in ASD and SPD using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). | Kruskal- Wallis tests | Small sample size |
Booules-Katri et al[84], 2019 | Patients and relatives of schizophrenia patients attending psychiatric service at a hospital in Spain; Public advertisements | To compare the ToM performance of a group of HFA and SSPD with a matched HC group | t-test | SSPD sample consisted of non-clinical individuals |
Ref. | Participants | Measures | PD assessment instrument | PD Prevalence (at least one PD) |
Ketelaars et al[43], 2008 | n = 15 (4 AS, 10 PDD-NOS, 1 HFA) | AQ, SCAN-2.1 | IPDE | > 50% |
Rydén and Bejerot[40], 2008 | n = 84 (5 autistic disorder, 51 AS, 28 PDD-NOS); 37% comorbid ADHD | SCID-I, WAIS III, ASSQ, ASDI, ASRS, MADRS,Y-BOCS, GAF, CGI-S, WRAADDS | SCID-II screen; SPP | > 40% |
Hofvander et al[14], 2009 | n = 117 (5 autistic disorder, 62 AS, 50 PDD-NOS) | WAIS-R or WAIS-IIISCID-I, ASDI | SCID-II | 62% |
Lugnegård et al[38], 2012 | n = 54 (AS) | WAIS-III, DISCOS-11AQ | SCID-II or a structured DSM-IV-based clinical interview | 48% |
Strunz et al[26], 2015 | n = 59 (49 AS, 10 HFA) | ADOS, ADI-R, MINI, SCID-I, DAPP-BQ,NEO-PI-R | SCID-II | 45% |
Geurts and Jansen[44], 2011 | n = 105 (27 autistic disorder, 28 AS, 50 PDD- NOS); 34% of sample with intellectual disability | Former DSM-IV Axis I diagnosis reported | Former DSM-IV Axis II diagnosis reported | 15% |
Anckarsäter et al[47], 2006 | n = 174 subjects with childhood onset neuropsychiatric disorder (47 ASD, 27 ASD+ADHD, 81 ADHD, 19 other diagnosis) | SCID-I, ASDI, Y-BOCS; ASHFAQ, TCI | SCID- II | 75% |
PD | Lugnegård et al[38], 2012 | Hofvander et al[14], 2009 | Anckarsäter et al[47], 2006 | Strunz et al[26], 2015 |
Paranoid | 0% | 19% | 25.5 % ASD; 25.9% ASD + ADHD | 2% |
Schizoid | 26% | 13% | 31.9% ASD; 22.2% ASD + ADHD | 36% |
Schizotypal | 2% | 21% | 23.4% ASD; 11.1% ASD + ADHD | 0% |
Antisocial | 0% | 3% | 0% ASD; 18.5% ASD + ADHD | 0% |
Histrionic | 0% | 0% | 0% | 0% |
Borderline | 0% | 9% | 10.6% ASD; 14.8% ASD + ADHD | 0% |
Narcissistic | 0% | 3% | 6.4%ASD; 3.7% ASD + ADHD | 0% |
Avoidant | 13% | 25% | 34% ASD; 11.1% ASD + ADHD | 2% |
Obsessive-compulsive | 19% | 32% | 42.6% ASD; 29.6% ASD + ADHD | 17% |
Dependent | 0% | 5% | 8.5% ASD; 22.2% ASD + ADHD | 0% |
Ref. | Participants | Comparison group | Measures | Personality measures | Results |
Anckarsäter et al[47], 2006 | n = 113 (6 autistic disorder, 46 AS, 66 Atypical Autism); 47ASD+ADHD 66 ASD | Age and sex matched group | SCID-I; ASDI; Y-BOCS; ASHFAQ; TCI | TCI; SCID-II | Lower NS, RD, SD, C; Higher HA; Cluster A and Cluster C PD were common |
Soderstrom et al[50], 2002 | n = 31 AS | Age and sex matched group | WAIS-III | TCI | Higher HA ST; Lower NS, RD, SD, C |
Sizoo et al[49], 2009 | n = 75 (53 without SUD, 8 with past SUD, 14 with current SUD) | n = 657 NC | ADI-R; ADOS; DSM-IV criteria checklists; WAIS-III | VTCI | Higher HA, ST; Lower RD, SD, C; Lower NS and RD for ASD without SUD; Higher P for subgroups with current or past SUD |
Vuijk et al[51], 2018 | n = 66 (15 ASD, 25 AS, 26 PDD-NOS) | Matched comparison group (age, education, marital status) | TCI | Higher HA, lower NS, RD, SD, C | |
Helles et al[52], 2016 | n = 40 AS | Within comparison group (no longer ASD/ASD pure/ASD plus) | GAFWAIS-IIIASDI; BDI; ASRS | TCI | Higher RD in no longer ASD; Higher HA, lower NS in ASD pure; Higher HA, lower C, SD in ASD plus |
Ref. | Partecipants | Comparison group | Measures | Personality trait measures | Results |
Schwartzman et al[56], 2016 | n = 364 adults with elevated ASD traits | n = 464 adults with lower ASD traits | RAADS-R | IPIP-NEO-120 | Neuroticism was positively correlated with ASD symptomatology; Extraversion, openness to experience, conscientiousness, and agreeableness were negatively correlated with ASD; About 70% of the variance in RAADS-R scores accounted for by the IPIP-NEO-120 facets. A great variability in personality traits emerged in the elevated ASD traits group with four distinct clusters of FFM personality types |
Schriber et al[55], 2014 | n = 37 ASD (29% HFA, 57% AS, 14% PDD-NOS) | n = 42 NC | WAIS; ADOS G | BFI | Higher Neuroticism Lower Openness to experience, Conscientiousness, Extraversion, Agreeableness |
Kanai et al[67], 2011 | n = 64 AS | n = 65 NC | AQ; HADS; L-SAS | NEO-FFI | AQ, HADS, and L-SAS were significantly higher in AS than in control. Higher Neuroticism, Lower Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness AQ correlated with the subscale scores of HADS and NEO-FFI in AS |
Strunz et al[26], 2015 | n = 59 ASD(83% AS, 17% HFA) | n = 62 NPD,80 BPD, 106 NC | SCID-I/MINI | NEO-PI-R; DAPP BQ; SCID-II | On the NEO-PI-R: Conscientiousness: NCC = ASD > BPD and NPD Neuroticism: NCC < ASD = NPD < BPD; Extraversion: ASD < BPD, NPD, NCCOpenness for experience: ASD < NCC, BPD, NPDAgreeableness: ASD = BPD and NPD > NCCOn the DAPP-BQ: Inhibitedness: ASD = BPD > NCC and NPD Dissocial Behaviour: NCC = ASD < BPD and NPD; Emotional dysregulation: NCC < ASD = NPD < BPD Compulsivity: ASD > BPD, NPD, NCC |
Hesselmark et al[62], 2015 | n = 48 ASD | n = 53 NC | MINI | NEOPI-R | Satisfactory internal consistency of the NEOPI-R. Neuroticism correlated with psychiatric comorbidity in ASD group |
Ref. | Participants | Comparison group | Measures | Personality measures | Results |
Ozonoff et al[65], 2005 | n = 20 HFA | 24 NC (age, intelligence and gender matched college students) | WAIS-R | MMPI-2 | Higher Depression, Social Introversion, Social Discomfort, Repression and PSY-5 scale Introversion |
Kanai et al[59], 2011 | n = 55 AS | 57 NC | WAIS-R | SPQEPQ | SPQ: AS>NC; SPQ subscale scores (unusual perceptual experiences, odd behaviour, and suspiciousness) were correlated with total scores of the AQ in the AS group; Higher ‘Neuroticism’ and ‘Psychoticism’; Lower ‘Extraversion’ and ‘Lie’ |
Ref. | Participants | Comparison group | Measures | Results |
Strunz et al[26], 2015 | 59 ASD (83% AS, 17% HFA) | 62 NPD, 80 BPD, 106 NC | NEO-PI-R; DAPP BQ; SCID-I/MINI; SCID-II | On the NEO-PI-R: Conscientiousness: NCC = ASD > BPD and NPD; Neuroticism: NCC < ASD = NPD < BPD; Extraversion: ASD < BPD, NPD, NCC; Openness for experience: ASD < NCC, BPD, NPD; Agreeableness: ASD = BPD and NPD > NCC; on the DAPP-BQ; Inhibitedness: ASD = BPD > NCC and NPD; Dissocial Behaviour: NCC = ASD < BPD and NPD; Emotional dysregulation: NCC < ASD = NPD < BPD; Compulsivity: ASD > BPD, NPD, NCC |
López-Pérez et al[95], 2017 | 30 AS | 30 BPD60 matched NC | SCID-ISCID-IIEmotion regulation of others and self (two scales: extrinsic affect improvement, extrinsic affect worsening)Interpersonal emotion management | Affect improvement: BPD = AS < NNC; Affect worsening: BPD = AS = NNC; Affect improvement > affect worsening in BPD e NCC; Affect improvement = affect worsening in ASD; Adaptive interpersonal strategies (attention deployment, cognitive change) ASD < BPD and NNC; Maladaptive interpersonal strategies (expressive suppression) ASD > BPD and control. |
Dudas et al[92], 2017 | 624 ASD | 23 BPD; 16 ASD+ BPD; 2081 NC | AQ; EQ; SQR; SCID-II | AQ: NC < BPD = ASC < ASC+BPD; EQ:NC = BPD > ASC = ASC+BPD; SQR NC < BPD = ASC = ASC+BPD |
Murphy[100]2006 | 39 AS; Male forensic patients detained in high security psychiatric care | 39 PD (antisocial and/or borderline)39 SC with positive symptoms detained in high security psychiatric care | WAIS-R; ToM measures | IQ PD = AS > SC; AS and SC performed worse on two ToM measures (the Revised Eyes Task and the second order mental representation stories) |
Stanfield et al[87], 2017 | 28 ASD | 21 SPD; 10 CM; 33 NC | ADOS-G; SCID-II; PANSS; WAISsocial judgment taskEkmann 60 facies task; fRMI task of social judgement | SPD = CM = ASD < controls on social judgment task and Ekman 60-Faces Test; on positive symptoms: ASD < SPD = CM; on negative symptoms ASD = SPD > CM; fRMI: hyperactivation in SPD and CM group compared to ASD was found in the amygdala and the cerebellum |
Booules-Katri et al[84], 2019 | 35 HFA | SSPD (n = 30) and a NC (n = 36) | O-LIFE questionnaire; SCID-I; SCID-II; ADI-R; ADOS; WAIS-III; ToM test | HFA showed greater impairment and no dissociation between affective and cognitive ToM components; SSPD scored significantly lower on cognitive than affective ToM test |
- Citation: Rinaldi C, Attanasio M, Valenti M, Mazza M, Keller R. Autism spectrum disorder and personality disorders: Comorbidity and differential diagnosis. World J Psychiatr 2021; 11(12): 1366-1386
- URL: https://www.wjgnet.com/2220-3206/full/v11/i12/1366.htm
- DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.5498/wjp.v11.i12.1366