Review Open Access
Copyright ©The Author(s) 2021. Published by Baishideng Publishing Group Inc. All rights reserved.
World J Orthop. Jun 18, 2021; 12(6): 360-375
Published online Jun 18, 2021. doi: 10.5312/wjo.v12.i6.360
Slacklining: A narrative review on the origins, neuromechanical models and therapeutic use
Charles Philip Gabel, Research Section, Access Physiotherapy, Coolum Beach 4573, Australia
Bernard Guy, Ecole des Mines de Saint-Etienne, Saint Etienne 4200, Loire, France
Hamid Reza Mokhtarinia, Department of Ergonomics and Physiotherapy, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran 12345, Iran
Markus Melloh, School of Health Professions, Institute of Health Sciences, Zurich University of Applied Sciences, Winterthur 8410, Switzerland
Markus Melloh, School of Medicine, The University of Western Australia, Perth WA 6009, Australia
Markus Melloh, Curtin Medical School, Curtin University, Bentley WA 6102, Australia
ORCID number: Charles Philip Gabel (0000-0001-8354-4545); Bernard Guy (0000-0002-0748-9369); Hamid Reza Mokhtarinia (0000-0002-5181-4894); Markus Melloh (0000-0002-8819-799X).
Author contributions: Gabel CP proposed the concept and outline; Melloh M provided critical input for the manuscript content with specific relevance to physiology, biopsychosocial health, clinical guidelines and current medical models, references and editing of the manuscript; Mokhtarinia HR provided critical input for the manuscript content with specific relevance to therapeutic and rehabilitation aspects, physiology, references and editing of the manuscript; Guy B provided specific vital input regarding the aspects of time and fundamental physics as well as referencing and editing of the manuscript; all authors contributed to writing the manuscript.
Conflict-of-interest statement: No author has a conflict of interest for this study.
Open-Access: This article is an open-access article that was selected by an in-house editor and fully peer-reviewed by external reviewers. It is distributed in accordance with the Creative Commons Attribution NonCommercial (CC BY-NC 4.0) license, which permits others to distribute, remix, adapt, build upon this work non-commercially, and license their derivative works on different terms, provided the original work is properly cited and the use is non-commercial. See: http://creativecommons.org/Licenses/by-nc/4.0/
Corresponding author: Charles Philip Gabel, BPhty, MSc, PhD, Physiotherapist, Research Scientist, Research Section, Access Physiotherapy, 12 Grandview DR, Coolum Beach 4573, Australia. cp.gabel@bigpond.com
Received: December 4, 2020
Peer-review started: December 4, 2020
First decision: January 24, 2021
Revised: January 27, 2021
Accepted: May 19, 2021
Article in press: May 19, 2021
Published online: June 18, 2021
Processing time: 189 Days and 7.8 Hours

Abstract

Slacklining, the neuromechanical action of balance retention on a tightened band, is achieved through self-learned strategies combining dynamic stability with optimal energy expenditure. Published slacklining literature is recent and limited, including for neuromechanical control strategy models. This paper explores slacklining’s definitions and origins to provide background that facilitates understanding its evolution and progressive incorporation into both prehabilitation and rehabilitation. Existing explanatory slacklining models are considered, their application to balance and stability, and knowledge-gaps highlighted. Current slacklining models predominantly derive from human quiet-standing and frontal plane movement on stable surfaces. These provide a multi-tiered context of the unique and complex neuro-motoric requirements for slacklining’s multiple applications, but are not sufficiently comprehensive. This consequently leaves an incomplete understanding of how slacklining is achieved, in relation to multi-directional instability and complex multi-dimensional human movement and behavior. This paper highlights the knowledge-gaps and sets a foundation for the required explanatory control mechanisms that evolve and expand a more detailed model of multi-dimensional slacklining and human functional movement. Such a model facilitates a more complete understanding of existing performance and rehabilitation applications that opens the potential for future applications into broader areas of movement in diverse fields including prostheses, automation and machine-learning related to movement phenotypes.

Key Words: Slacklining; Neuromechanics; Human movement; Model; Balance; Rehabilitation

Core Tip: Slacklining is an ancient activity; however, modern scientific literature is very recent and limited. This paper explores slacklining’s origins to provide background on its evolution and incorporation into prehabilitation and rehabilitation. Existing mechanical models and neurophysiological explanations are considered, summarised, and their applications and knowledge-gaps highlighted. Consequently, the need for improved understanding and descriptive and mathematical models are highlighted to ensure a multi-tiered understanding of slacklining’s unique and complex neuro-motoric requirements for its multiple applications, including human functional movement. With understanding slacklining’s history and fundamentals comes the potential for future broader applications for functional movement, prosthesis, automation, and machine-learning.



INTRODUCTION

Slacklining is defined as a complex neuromechanical task involving achievement of functional independence while maintaining dynamic stability through balance retention. This occurs from the interactions of the individual’s whole-body where internal dynamics drive the response to external environmental changes, while treading on an unstable, three-dimensional moveable, tightened, webbing-band fixed at each end[1,2]. The concepts explored in this paper consider and address what slacklining is, and its historical origins, in order to provide essential background understanding of the activity and its evolution, recognition and progressive incorporation into prehabilitation and rehabilitation. With this background knowledge, the existing explanatory models can be considered, how they apply to an individual’s capacity to achieve stability, and whether they provide a complete recognition and understanding of the explanatory control mechanisms for multi-directional instability and complex multi-dimensional movement. As such, they should be able to explain slacklining as a harmonious functional movement (HFM), namely the integrated inter-relation of neural and muscular components to facilitate and enable a stable harmonized full-body functional movement[3]. Examining these current models enables the determination and recognition that there are knowledge-gaps, which indicates that existing models must be evolved and expanded in order to encompass the broad conceptual circumstances that achieve slacklining stability.

THE ORIGINS AND ACTIONS OF SLACKLINING

As a trend-sport, modern slacklining is an adaptation of the ancient performing art of traditional rope-walking. Modern slacklining was initially started by climbers and outdoor enthusiasts in the 1960s and 1970s in the European Alps of Switzerland, France and Austria as well as in Yosemite National Park in the United States of America, where ropes and cables were replaced with adapted lightweight webbing and ratchet technology that incorporated an easier, safer and more elastic line[4,5]. Slacklining was originally termed “line-walking” or “funambulaire” (the precursor term to slacklining) in Latin European regions and “Jultagi” or “Eoreum” in Korea as a representation of Central Asia[5,6]. Slacklining has very ancient, established history and cultural traditions. The earliest records are insinuated as being possible from approximately 40000 years Before Christ (BC) when man first invented ropes[7]. Consequently, the necessity and challenge arose to walk along these ropes to enable the completion of normal tasks in construction, movement along and between habitation sites such as cliffs and trees[8], for communication and social interaction such as crossing gorges as continues in the Russian Dagestan region, on the ropes of early Phoenician sailing vessels in ancient Crete around 1000 BC, and eventually for entertainment[4,9]. More direct inference is made at approximately 20000 BC where, “line-walking” or “funambulaire” was an established societal activity in pre-ancient Greece, China and the Korean Peninsula[4,6,10]. The earliest documentation appears to be in pottery and figurines of Cycladic acrobats from around 3000 BC[11]; and as a performance component of the ancient Greek Olympics from 776 BC and earlier Greek Games where the “Thaumatronwas awarded to those who performed a marvelous act or spectacle[6].

However, historical writings suggest the ancient Olympics were an extension of games from earlier periods, particularly the bronze age of Anatolia (3000-2500 BC), Crete (3000-1100 BC) and Mycenae (1600-1100), with some archaeological data suggesting games dated back to the 10th century BC[12]. Funambulaire was an integral social performing art from at least 3000 BC[4,6]; and demonstrated in the fresco paintings of Selini funambulist rope-walkers from the Villa of Cicero in Pompeii’s ash covered ruins estimated as approximately 100-50 BC[13]. The natural progression from ropes and cables to ratchets and webbing reflects human evolutionary capacity found in multiple incorporations and replacements of traditional and established items into modern product developments and activities[8]. The graded incorporation into rehabilitation could have been initiated at any time, but medical literary writings insinuate at least around 150 Anno Domini (AD) during the period of Imperial Roman (31BC-AD476)[10,14], while the first published papers were in European sports science literature around 2009[15,16] (Figure 1).

Figure 1
Figure 1  Historical progressions: Rope walking to slacklining and use in rehabilitation. BC: Before Christ; AD: Anno Domini.
SLACKLINING: EXERCISE, REHABILITATION AND THERAPY
Slacklining in exercise and rehabilitation

The incorporation of such a fundamental yet physically complex recreational or cultural activity into the fields of therapy and rehabilitation is another evolutionary reflection. Slacklining, as therapy, has only been formally investigated and documented in the published scientific literature over the last decade[15-17]. There is anecdotal evidence of slacklining’s therapeutic use since the 1970’s and 1980’s as both prehabilitation and rehabilitation for Alpine sports and athletes. This includes Ingemar Stenmark for downhill slalom ski[15], within Australia’s competitive surfing community and elite training programs that led to the high performance center at Casuarina on the Australian East Coast[2], and for rock-climbing[17,18]. However, its first recorded application may be inferred from the Greek-Roman physician Galen-of-Pergamon from 150-200 AD. Galen is recognized as the father of “Exercise in Medicine”[10,19] with over 500 medical Treatises[20] that provide potential documentation for the use of rope walking for rehabilitation and prehabilitation purposes. The texts considered[21-23] do not mention rope walking specifically as an activity, but such an activity or exercise is highly likely to have been used and can be insinuated from the following knowledge: rope walking was, as mentioned, an accepted and integral activity within Imperial Roman Society[4,6]; Galen is considered the “father of rehabilitation” who advocated the importance of exercise, his texts and teaching were the recognized authority for almost 1600 years[24], particularly for exercise and health[22,25]. Galen wrote extensively on knee injuries including loss of function and balance in athletes and Gladiators, noting that knee muscle control required considerable time to regain due to loss of muscle activation, with “wasting in the absence of local muscle or nerve damage[24], a reference to arthrogenic muscle inhibition (AMI)[6,11,14,18]. Galen prescribed exercise for rehabilitation and prehabilitation as essential for “… health and good condition”[26], and ”… strength for function”[19,22]. Consequently, though not specific in detail, slacklining could have occurred through the informal or guided inclusion of an existing, recognized and challenging balance activity. Consequently, the incorporation of slacklining into rehabilitation may not be “that” recent, but actually date back some 2000 years.

Slacklining and arthrogenic muscle inhibition

The discussion of AMI is an important consideration as slacklining appears to negate the local muscular inhibitory action, particularly in the knee quadriceps[2] and potentially in the spinal lumbar multifidi[27]. AMI is defined as muscular activation failure, about a damaged or distended joint, due to ongoing neural activation deficit[28,29]. AMI has been well recognised since at least the 19th century as having a central-neural basis[30], being a consequence of articular sensory receptor discharge from joint afferents[31] due to swelling, inflammation, joint laxity[32] and tissue damage. The presence of AMI is consistent across different joints and joint pathologies, particularly the knee[29,30,32,33], hip[34], elbow[35], shoulder[31] ankle[36] and intersegmental spinal muscles[37]. Evidence indicates that with AMI, supra-spinal pathways potentially play a significant role through inhibitory down regulation that affects central nervous system (CNS) neural inhibition, that in-turn prevents full muscle activation. This is in conjunction with spinal reflex contributions through the Group-I non-reciprocal inhibitory pathway reflexes including the flexion and Gamma (γ)-loop[28]. Today, the full understanding of AMI remains incomplete and is the subject of significant investigative research[38]. However, slacklining’s recognized effect in over-riding this down regulation, in the lumbo-pelvic[27] and lower limb regions[2,18] is gradually being recognized and is a significant advancement in anatomical and condition specific regional rehabilitation[39,40].

Slacklining as therapy

The therapeutic direction of slacklining has evolved over the past decade to become an adjunct in both injury prevention, such as falls in the elderly[41], and specific sports including judo[42], basketball[43], badminton[44], handball[45], and football/soccer[46]; as well as in rehabilitation[2], including orthopedics[2,27], neurology[27,39,40], sports training[47], general physical training[46] performance[43], and recreation[5]. This inclusion of slacklining with other prevention and rehabilitation themes derives from the triad of sensory system contributors of proprioception, vision, and vestibular somatosensory inputs[48]. This is a consequence of the unique properties of slacklining[2] due to its distinct action from other conventional balance activities[49] and apparatus[41,43] as a composite-chain activity, i.e. there is a weak link in the kinetic chain resulting in abnormal motor synergy patterns due to the contact surface of the loaded limbs having free, partially supported but unstable movement on a recoil resistance surface[50]. The coupling of these qualities and the CNS contributors results in slacklining having four integrated qualities[2]: Balance - the equilibrium control regulating the body’s segmental dynamic movement and center-of-mass (CoM) within the base of support[51]; postural control - the body's positional control in space[52]; muscle strength - the muscular generated forces[43,53]; and neuromechanical demand - the integration of neurobiology and biomechanics[1].

MATHEMATICAL AND GENERAL MODELS THAT EXPLAIN SLACKLINING
Manifold models

It is recognised that the neuromechanical control strategies employed during slacklining are not fully understood[54]. Several detailed proposed models seek to explain slacklining and predominantly utilise a conceptual “manifold model”. However, the term manifold has two denotations: firstly, within a general context as a “conceptual dimension inside a general discussion”; and secondly, within a mathematical context as a “mathematical subset with lower dimension inside a mathematical model”[55]. The latter context is a defined shaped space where all activity and analysis occurs[56], which for slacklining likely simulates a saddle-shape within ordinary task space[57]. This saddle-manifold is shaped concave in the dimensions of the direction of the slackline (X) and convex in the direction of its lateral shift (Y) and the dimension of gravity or sag (Z)[58,59].

Scientifically and mathematically a “manifold has the same or lower dimensionality as the underlying mathematical model, though mathematically the number of dimensions possible is arbitrary. Each point within the manifold has its own “homeomorphic” space. Consequently, a “bi-continuous function” exists between all points being on both a continuum in a given direction, while concurrently having an opposing continuous inverse function[60]. The manifold model concept was initially proposed within robotics as the “self-motion manifold”[61], and, subsequently applied to human movement and functional tasks as the “un-controlled manifold” (UCM) model[62]. Such a model suitably explains quiet-standing[58,59] and simple functional frontal plane movements e.g., walking and postural sway[57,63], but was not proposed for complicated tasks involving dynamic contact, e.g., slacklining[1,54].

The UCM may still, however, provide a potential solution as it proposes a motor control strategy for redundant systems by using the abundant solutions inherent to them[62]. This postulates that the task redundant space of the effector is not homogeneous, but structured according to task requirements[61,64]. Consequently, the UCM model may be expanded to accommodate slacklining as the increased variability and redundancy found in such challenging actions can be redistributed, allowing acceptable levels of task variability to be maintained[56,65].

In human quiet-standing and frontal plane movement on stable surfaces, the Newtonian equations in classical mechanics of an inverted pendulum are generally considered acceptable mechanical models for self-balancing[51,59,66]. These considerations are critical in acknowledging howthe body estimates its current orientation in space, in order to, subsequently, generate the corrective stabilizing actions required to maintain what is a mechanically unstable upright stance. However, for slacklining, this model is not suitable. The base is not fixed: Consequently, body neuromechanical dynamics are coupled with the slackline’s external dynamics, and the natural actions and subsequent responses of body-sway[1,54,67].

Application of the manifold model to slacklining

To consider these attributes within a global encompassing model to describe slacklining as a dynamic and changing series of quantitative factors, Paoletti et al[1] proposed that “an optimal strategy is achieved for self-balance. This is through the consideration of a nonlinear model that accounts for potential parameter coupling and overall CNS performance delays that occur on multiple time scales. These can be visualized as a system with passive coupled dynamics working in unison with sensory modalities, enabling orientation and activated motor coordination of the body to be self-directed or “inferred”. In Serrien et al[68], where the balance strategy was examined within an “expanded manifold of a higher dimension” that the subjects must remain within, this approach in turn enabled the model to account for coordination from the perspective of “self-organizing maps”[68]. This model-modification allowed an entire kinematic chain response where equilibrium is achieved as an integrated CNS solution to the presented task-environment-subject dependent situation and allows for a large set of degrees of freedom (DoF) that facilitate understanding of postural control within the human motor system[69]. Additional perspectives from Vallery et al[53] clarify that these neuromuscular actions involve a decoupling of the stance leg/s, and its produced vector forces from the residual body, through muscular co-contraction at the hip. This is augmented by arm movement and bimanual arm coordination[70] that act as the primary initial compensatory reaction to CoM displacement[47], and in turn the arms concurrently provide angular momentum influences[54].

BALANCE AND HARMONIOUS FUNCTIONAL MOVEMENT

In the balance model for achieving slackline independence through human HFM, the manifold’s defined space contains variables with a significant set of DoF that contribute to particular movements or functions[69], some of which provide the amalgamation of stability and mobility[3] through CNS control[56]. Consequently, the model’s manifold contains both stable controlled variables and unstable un-controlled variables, where no action control is required as a task-variable’s position is not affected[62]. Any given set of CNS controlled DoF that provide stability, consequently, have variables separated into two orthogonal/opposing-directional subspaces: one with actions that have no effect (on the controlled variables); the other with orthogonal-subspace actions that do[62]. If variability in the controlled subspace is smaller than that of the opposing orthogonal-subspace, then the CNS, and, consequently, stability control, is unaffected[56]. These manifold models simplify reality[1] and enable a mathematical representation and analysis. They also provide insight into what CNS control aspects are present when postural balance strategies are implemented[71,72], and that all responses occur within physically defined boundary limitations[47].

SUMMARY

The outlined existing hypothetical models of slacklining describe the neuromechanical control strategies that achieve this activity, predominantly through use of a conceptual “manifold” that simulates a “saddle-shaped” task space[58,59]. Through the use of classic mechanics this is envisaged as a self-balancing mechanical model of an inverted pendulum[51,66]. These concepts critically acknowledge spatial orientation and subsequently corrective stabilization actions. However, this is generally hypothesized around a stable or fixed base which is inadequate for a body dynamically coupled with external changes and response actions[1,54,67]. Consequently, there is a knowledge-gap and a required evolution and expansion of these ideas, not only from the perspective of mechanics[1,51,54], but also from that of the neurological[73] and biopsychosocial[74] constraints of the individual.

PHYSICAL FORCES AND ORDINARY SPACE
Existing models of harmonious functional movement

Models of human HFM and self-balancing generally consider two concurrent spheres of input: firstly, mathematical mechanical precision models bound by classical mechanics equations and; secondly, supplementary sensory and motoric neural control[47,67]. The mechanical models were initiated over the last 600 years, most likely from Da Vinci’s documented model proposition: motion is created by the destruction of balance, that is, of equality of weight, for nothing can move by itself which does not leave its state of balance, and that thing moves most rapidly which is furthest from its balance[75]. In contrast, the importance and relevance of the control aspects of a neural source were not proposed till the mid to late 1800’s, when the understanding of sensory input and neural latency were detailed in seminal German publications.

The initial aspects of sensory control include the perspective of frames of reference as used by the nervous system, and the kinematic constraints that these place upon any control aspect. Initially, this came from the role of vision and was established from Donders[75]’ Law, namely “that there is a unique orientation of the eye when looking in any specific direction”[76]. This was supplemented by Listing's law which “… specifies what this orientation is”[77,78], being a principle that governs eye movements through the three dimensional (3D) planes of horizontal, vertical, and torsional, and is actively implemented by a neural mechanism[79]. Fick[79] proposed that Listing’s law enhanced motor efficiency by “minimizing the rotational eccentricity of the eye”[80]. This sensory-motoric control system was quantified by von Helmholtz[80] who measured the time delay that occurs due to the finite speed of neural signal transmission, where response time is dependent on the input-output delays of single neurons and neural chains. Donders[81] furthered these control phenomena in his work related to “The speed of mental processing”, which determined a relationship was present between an individual’s “choice-response” and the number of stimuli present[82]. This was, subsequently, supported and progressed by Merkel[82] who defined that “the response time is longer when a stimulus belongs to a larger set of stimuli”. This eventually led to Hick’s Law[83], or the Hick-Hyman Law almost 100 years later that proposed “…information is intimately concerned with reaction time” and describes “… the rate of gain of information”, and assesses the involvement of “cognitive capacity”[84].

The Hick-Hyman Law denotes that the time an individual requires for decision-making results from the choices available, and will increase logarithmically with the number of choices. In slacklining, these choices increase dramatically compared to simple frontal plane movements[54,57,63]. Consequently, transmission rates vary significantly between cell types and transmission mediums being affected by neuron size, the presence of axon myelination and its composition[85], sheath number, length, thickness and distribution[86], the size and distance between nodes of Ranvier, types of intervening synapses and how many occur between the CNS and peripheral receptors, and the stimuli strength, and quality[85,87]. The sphere of task control and the time-delay present is due to the sensory and motoric neural pathways necessary signaling processes, such that delay increases as the system and its requirements become more complicated[58,73]. This accounts for the balance organ with the integrated labyrinthine and vision systems that are adapted to account for these time delays[67], and ensure the essential limitations necessary for the human body to remain stable[64].

This is particularly relevant for more complex movements, such as those that occur on unstable surfaces, at higher speeds and require greater skill, with slacklining being such an example. Accordingly, in human movement, the more complex the task and the more stimuli present, the slower the action[17,88] or the greater the response time, Fitts’s[88] Law (Table 1), and the higher the neuronal firing rate[1]. Further considerations are the large variation in effects of normal and pathological ageing[85] on human mechanical structures[67,86], neural system time delays[64]; and that injured tissue and individuals at the global level (such as neurological conditions) will not be able to achieve the required levels of adaptation, and, consequently, the individual will exhibit postural deficiency or balance loss[53,89,90]. The overall consequence is a simplified large-scale time-delayed model that provides insight from classical mechanics into the integrated functioning of the body systems and organs that support the theories and hypotheses of “integrated control”[48,67].

Table 1 Glossary of definitions and explanation of scientific terms.
Scientific Term
Explanation/definition
Fitts LawTime to accomplish movement linearly increases with the logarithm of the index of the task difficulty
Hopf bifurcation pointA critical point where a system's stability switches and a periodic solution arises
Two-thirds Power LawExpresses the robust local relationship between the geometrical and temporal aspects of human movement
Elliptic geometryNon-Euclidean (or non-ordinary) geometry stating that there are no lines parallel to any given line, this is an example of Riemannian geometry
Affine TransformationsA transformation that preserves lines and parallelism
Equi-affine Transformationsa transformation that preserves areas, in addition to lines and parallelism
Temporal SegmentationThe central or brain action of breaking down motion sequences into different actions
Isochrony PrincipleThe duration of voluntary movement remains approximately constant across a range of movement distances; that is, movement duration is independent of movement extent
Kinematic redundancyKinematic redundancy occurs when a manipulator has more degrees of freedom than those strictly required to execute a given task. Additional active joints and interlinked segments improve both mobility and the available degrees of freedom
Inter-segmental law of coordinationA kinematic law that describes the coordination patterns among the elevation angles of the lower limb segments during locomotion (Borghese). It is reliant on accurate progressive timing of muscular contractions in adjacent segments and appendages

The subsequent progressions in the mathematical mechanical human movement and self-balancing models came at the start of last century when Graham-Brown[90] reported on “dynamic principles involved in progression”. He stated that: “The cycle of progression may be supposed to commence at a point at which one of the limbs is perpendicular to the ground. The “initial velocity” then carries the body past this point, and it then falls forwards along the circumference of a circle the radius of which is the limb in contact with the ground”. Over the last half century there has been further evolution and progression with Bresler et al[91] who remarked that the “Dynamic balance of the “head, arms and trunk” about the supporting hip is dependent upon the control of pelvic motion by the hip musculature”; while Saunders et al[92] recognised the significance of “Pelvic lateral tilt (being) identified as one of the primary determinants of gait”.

In 1971, Adams[93] proposed the Classical model of “…a closed-loop theory for learning simple movements”, that incorporated feedback, error detection and error correction as key elements. This model required that the output of the system had feedback, and compared the reference for error detection and, if necessary corrected for this to provide the resulting movement, such that it was “… self-regulating by compensating for deviating from the reference”. More recently, MacKinnon et al[50] described “Control of whole body balance in the frontal plane during human walking” through the model of the “inverted pendulum” which relies on the principles of equations from classical mechanics. This model was progressed through a series of evolving modifications to account for the influence of both random disturbance and control torque. This neurologically controlled delay is effectively present at the ankle joint due to the concurrent relationship between: the passive stiffness from the visco-elastic nature of the muscle-tendon-ligament complex; and the active modulating influence of regional muscles[73]. This is supplemented by the triceps surae muscles that maintains balance by ”predictively controlling the proximal offset of the spring-like element in a ballistic-like manner”[94,95].

Within the mechanical model it is also critical to consider that the inherently unstable upright position, where the smallest deviation eliminates equilibrium, is retained through uniformity between retarding and controlling forces[67,96]. These provide contributing components that can be low-level, such as passive ankle stiffness, vs high level, such as the growth-rate of the gravitational toppling torque[58,95]. Together these forces are considered a simple closed-loop control model[48] as a progression of Adams “closed loop” model[94]. A further mechanical model input consideration integrates neural sensory aspects, where feedback mechanisms are based predominantly on body-sway motion[63] from balance perturbations[97]. These evoke “sensory weights”, a form of neural control representing comparative contributions of each sensory systems and integrated as a “package”[48]. This provides an internal estimate of orientation[71], where the assessed and adjusted responses are determined by how these inputs contribute to the total balance system as a single component[1]. Consequently, this “package” is itself dynamic and varies sufficiently to ensure equilibrium and prevent instability from corrective actions being either over- or under-produced[48]. The inputs themselves therefore exhibit bi-fold competing variations, due to the quality of the sensory information received. This is a consequence of changes in: (1) The external environmental conditions; and (2) The internal conditions affected by injury, neurologic disorders[90,98], or other psycho-social factors[65,74]. In the presence of a moving platform, as with slacklining, the vestibular and somatic sensory feedback is more highly weighted; while in quiet-standing the proprioceptive and visual systems are dominant[71]. Further, during afferent motor control the CNS creates muscle synergies” where groups of muscles are combined as a common neural signal to control a range of movements which can be modulated differently by each individual, consequently demonstrating the neuromuscular capacity for adaptive strategies to facilitate stability while slacklining[99]. This reinforces the findings of neuroplastic change, automatic or “packaged” signals[48,71], engrams[100] and homunculus smudging[37], all of which occur as a response to complex, demanding, balance challenge activities[100], which will, consequently, include slacklining[2,101].

The consequential reactive motoric action that produces the stability, in the mechanical paradigm perspective, is achieved through the generation of joint torques that appropriately correct for any deviations from a desired orientation[48,72]. The original perturbation changes are detected primarily by inputs from the visual, proprioceptive, and vestibular sensory sub-systems[67,71], with the resultant reflex delay[1,72] enabling a “Hopf bifurcation point”[2] (Table 1). This achieves a temporary, though dynamic, “solutionfor equilibrium and self-balance, with suitable active control strategies[58,67]. This “equilibrium solutionis explained by both the mechanical and sensory aspects within a saddle-like” 3D phase-space that is characterized by geometrical properties and spatial relations of position and velocity[58]. It is achieved through the optimal parity between two competing manifolds: one which is stable along the saddle and provides slow convergent motion in a direction toward the equilibrium; and the other which is unstable and provides divergent spiral motions such as pitch, roll, and yaw[97] about the axis of the phase planes, which further increase the number of dimensions and DoF present, as these actions occur in directions away from the equilibrium[58]. This “solutionis overseen by an intermittent proportional and derivative feedback system controller, that is characterized by the switching function at the “Hopf bifurcation point” in such a way that the proportional and derivative control is off” when the net force is near the stable manifold of the saddle and is on in all other circumstances. This controller is able to use considerably smaller regional space and feedback parameters, which make it more robust[58] in the designated phase plane, as it exhibits the “two-thirds power law”[3] (Table 1) scaling regimes typically found in physiological sway movements in humans. Concurrently, the nominal equilibrium state is itself surrounded by a “dead zone” that reflects the time-delay from the sensory neural control inputs[72] that will allow a motoric reaction[64] that provides spontaneous movement patterns, like sway, to occur[95]. As a simple feedback loop system, this can provide an acceptable model for dynamic stability to ensue[52,58]. It also accounts for: Age-related differential findings due to physiological change - such as passive joint stiffness and damping from osseous degeneration, and proprioceptive delay, which affect center-of-pressure-based sway behaviors[57,66]; plus notably increased “noise” from larger amplitude, plus false and extraneous input signals[57,73] that can all be viewed from a “classical mechanics influence perspective[67].

Any integrated mechanical and sensory model must also consider and discuss optimization approaches occurring, concurrently, within both aspects of the model, and that account for the “smoothness” or “flow” of HFM[3,102], e.g., compare a curve joining multiple points to a series of straight lines. This smooth, HFM is a consequence of the process of “speed-curvature coupling”[103] which is optimized by the coordinated brain action of sensory and motoric control under the influence of pre-planning and “temporal segmentation”, the process that “…identifies motion breakpoints and separates the different constituent phases”[104]. These sensory considerations are based on the mechanical assumptions of Riemannian or elliptic geometry[4,103] (Table 1). There is also a need to consider geometrical transformations that are both “affine”[5] (Table 1) and “equi-affine”[6] (Table 1), particularly in unstable settings, such as on a slackline, where the involved rotation, translation and shearing actions are both achieved and minimized in order to provide constant speed[102], such as occurs in the jerk response or Hoffman reflex[105].

This is of significant importance in relation to sensory input, particularly visual processing, as it confirms that “temporal segmentation”[7] (Table 1) of movement control complies with “equi-affine geometry”[103]. This geometrical transformation consequently accounts for the “two-thirds power law”, which governs the relationship between the geometrical and temporal parameters of human movement. This is centrally represented in “motion-planning”[61] and “human vision processing” and ensures that the “Isochrony principle”[8] (Table 1) is upheld[103]. This, consequently, allows for the application of differentiable manifolds of higher dimensions, an important consideration for complex activities on unstable mobile surfaces, such as slacklining. The consideration of these additional aspects within a mechanical model clarifies that human locomotion complies with the principles of “Kinematic redundancy[9] (Table 1); and the “Inter-segmental law of coordination”[10,103] (Table 1).

Together, these multiple aspects and considerations form the sensory and motoric neural control sphere, as noted previously from 18th Century German scientists, concepts that were, subsequently, integrated with the mechanical sphere. In unison they explain functional independence and balance control as the aforementioned closed-loop feedback system[94], with parity between the different integrated sensory orientation source information on one hand, and system feedback control constraints on the other[48]. However, such a conceptual model and presentation of balance, functional independence, and postural control, is only truly valid on stable surfaces[58]. When unstable surfaces, such as a slackline, are considered in such models, then further descriptive initiates, progressions, and evolutions are required[1,54] to account for the dynamic changing postures and positions[2,27,106]. This has led to the concept of “dimensionally-bound” manifold models, which require that the size-volume of the manifold is expanded. This accounts for the patterns of movement, control and balance-retention, that require higher levels of complexity within the significant DoF available for human motor system postural control[54].

THE CURRENT DESCRIPTIVE MODEL OF SLACKLINING

The concept of functional independence and self-balance being explained by dimensionally bound models, with expanded size-volume, takes into account activities such as slacklining that are significant progressions from those found in quiet-standing and simple frontal plane walking. This complexity occurs through the forces being produced more rapidly with the requirement for greater control occurring through smaller, more precise action-reaction dissipation[68]. There is a simultaneous reduction in both frequency and velocity in most available DoF, coupled with increased control of range of motion (RoM)[69]. This expanded manifold model allows greater distance in the Y and Z dimensions for any given X dimension position, but is limited by the physical constraints of the given slackline length (X dimension), its width and how far it can be moved laterally (Y Dimension) and the elastic-stretch that enables an optimal sag (Z Dimension). Consequently, a minimal effort or energy expenditure must be achieved and utilized to maintain stability in the presence of any perturbation[47]. This is consistent with the strategies of quiet-standing and stable surface walking[70]. However, any perturbation must be limited within the Y Axis, or lateral component of movement, to a distance in the order of 10 cm, for novices to moderate skilled slackliners, if control and balance on the line is to be retained[47]. Expert slackliners appear to exhibit greater lateral movement limitations within an expanded manifold[69].

This 3D model form has been envisaged and detailed with mathematical equations to validate positions[1,58]. However, to compound the descriptive difficulty of the model, it must be recognized that the optimal adopted strategy is individually selected. It can only be one of two methods prior to the introduction of any perturbation: (1) Two feet on the slackline, including use of an unloaded limb to touch and stabilize the line; or (2) One foot[2,5], with the other not touching but as a counter-balance that indirectly affects the manifold of the contacted and weight bearing foot[47]. In recognition of this deficiency the model was subsequently modified[54] through introducing a regional decoupling at the hips of the arms and trunk from the legs. This then accounted for the discrepancy that action-reaction forces generated by the arms, trunk, or free leg, could influence the point of contact, without moving the foot or changing the anterior-posterior direction by virtue of sheer forces, and ensures the principle of “energy optimization” was maintained[54]. This then allowed for individual joint torque and segmental interplay, where there is task distribution between the legs as the ultimate dissipater of the force vectors via the CoP dynamics at the foot/feet, but decoupled at the hips from the trunk and arms that influence, but do not control, angular momentum[47].

This 3D model then has the context that simulates the following example: an inverted pendulum[51] (the person), mounted on a cart (the slackline), that is moving on a circular or elliptical track[1] (the physical finite 3D ordinary space limitations of possible slackline movement), to a maximal distance from the slackline contact point[47] during instantaneous stability. Concurrently, the segments of the arms and trunk facilitate angular momentum control, and are decoupled by muscular co-contraction at the hips from the stance leg/s, that dynamically direct the vector forces[54].

The acquired movement control to achieve balance on a slackline is a set of self-learned patterns[96]. These form an instantaneous but fluid and interactive dynamic saddle-shaped manifold[58] that the individual must remain within to ensure stability[69] through balance control[66]. Stability is retained by the production of forces that are pro-active and re-active to those of the slackline’s 3D reciprocal forces at and through a point/s of fixed contact with the body via the foot[47] or feet[47]. Consequently, the slackline’s reactionary forces line of action always passes through the subject’s contact foot/feet and cannot be influenced without a dissipated moment from the force vector. This force, via the foot to the leg, is in the X-dimension or anterior-posterior direction[54], the Y-dimension of lateral deviation within the physical limit of the slackline’s topographical space[47], and the Z-dimension of available sag or reactionary bounce[107]. These forces form within the “high-dimensional manifold” “… in which the subjects have to stay in order to maintain balance”[68].

The expansion of this manifold can be learnt, with the improved balance then interpreted as enabling an increased RoM before the manifold edges are reached and balance is lost. Because the expanded manifold’s edge is now further, the frequency of movement change is lower, as the need to adapt or change is less, which in-turn leads to a reduced velocity of motion and less kinetic energy being required when reaching the manifold’s edge. Consequently, maintaining balance is easier and the energy used to counter the perturbation is dissipated in a stepwise manner[47,97].

FACTORS THAT NECESSITATE CHANGES TO THE CLASSICAL MECHANICS MANIFOLD MODEL

Currently, the models of HFM and self-balance are limited to those described above. They can account for all available DoFs as found with dynamic unpredictable movement on unstable surfaces that consider and account for kinematic redundancy in consort with inter-segmental coordination.

This can be summarised as follows: An integrated mathematical mechanical precision model, bound by classical mechanics equations in parity with sensory and motoric neural controls that approximates a dynamic “saddle-like” phase-space, high-dimensional manifold characterized by two internal competing manifolds of convergent and divergent motion.

This hypothetical model facilitates understanding the mechanisms for independent HFM and balance. These mechanical and neural components increase their complexity when considering the influence of “qualitative-implicit-knowledge”, such as the physics of gravity[108]. Generally, the brain hypothetically uses a qualitative internal model that incorporates classical mechanics-based equations from its existing knowledge of gravity and time as a multi-faceted perspective that include a quantified unit and both a spatial and cortical experience, even if this knowledge is quantitatively based[108]. This supplements sensory information when estimating more complex activities that involve external components, and not simply a body activity in isolation, including interceptive actions like hitting or catching[109]. However, when performing a “catch” in zero-gravity, movement is initiated earlier[109] as “qualitative-implicit-knowledge” is relied upon to pre-empt and approximate interceptive timing through pre-information that compensates for “no-gravity”[108]. This is analogous to core muscle pre-activation when using a limb. Further, “qualitative-implicit-knowledge” is influenced by the individual’s psychological state, motivation level, past activity-specific experience, the social context where the activity is performed and the presence of positive support or negative perceived social pressure to participate or “perform”[53,74]. Current movement models do not accommodate or acknowledge these additional aspects. By considering a highly skilled complex activity, such as slacklining as opposed to quiet standing, the intent is to consequently, recognize the areas of knowledge-gap so an evolved and expanded hypothetical model and paradigm can be proposed.

CONCLUSION

The successful achievement strategy that enables an individual to balance and remain on a slackline is currently explained as: A saddle shaped manifold model, where a moving inverted pendulum is subjected to self-generated and environmental forces within a defined 3D space, where the arm and trunk segments provide dynamic force influences but are decoupled from the hips and legs. This model integrates with gravity and the slackline’s elastic reactive properties being governed by classical mechanics. However, there remains a knowledge-gap as this dynamic stability is transitory, momentary, and acquired through the integration of additional dimensions not hereto considered. Consequently, within the context of historical and contemporary slacklining, the existing models are insufficient for the required explanatory control mechanisms that can accommodate multi-directional instability and complex multi-dimensional human HFM. This knowledge-gap requires an evolved and expanded model in order to provide an explanation for the control mechanisms in place. This evolved model will represent multiple, integrated dimensions that will facilitate the understanding of existing performance and incorporate general and rehabilitation activity. With such a model these applications can be applied more broadly in the future to movement in diverse fields that would potentially include prostheses, mechanized automation, altered gravity, robotics, mechatronics, artificial-intelligence-driven movement and machine-learning related to movement phenotypes.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

The author group acknowledges and specifically thank Dr. Ben Serrien for his invaluable input in the areas of physics, classical and quantum mechanics and assistance in manuscript editing. Without his input this article would not be completed.

Footnotes

Manuscript source: Invited manuscript

Corresponding Author's Membership in Professional Societies: Australian Physiotherapy Association, No. 6214.

Specialty type: Rehabilitation

Country/Territory of origin: Australia

Peer-review report’s scientific quality classification

Grade A (Excellent): 0

Grade B (Very good): B

Grade C (Good): 0

Grade D (Fair): 0

Grade E (Poor): 0

P-Reviewer: Byeon H S-Editor: Zhang L L-Editor: A P-Editor: Xing YX

References
1.  Paoletti P, Mahadevan L. Balancing on tightropes and slacklines. J R Soc Interface. 2012;9:2097-2108.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 30]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 32]  [Article Influence: 2.7]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
2.  Gabel CP. Slacklining: A novel exercise to enhance quadriceps recruitment, core strength and balance control. J Nov Physiother. 2014;4.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 2]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 3]  [Article Influence: 0.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
3.  Hoffman J, Gabel CP. The origins of Western mind-body exercise methods. Phys Ther Rev. 2015;20:315-324.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 8]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 7]  [Article Influence: 0.9]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
4.  Leckert O  An abridged history of funambulists. Electronic Article, 2014. [cited 20 March 2021]. Available from: https://www.atlasobscura.com/articles/an-abridged-history-of-funambulists.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
5.  Ashburn H  How to slackline. Helena Montana: Roman and Littlefield, 2013. [cited 20 March 2021]. Available from: https://xueshu.baidu.com/usercenter/paper/show?paperid=c62e43df190ee082bcf46389bfd4afdb&site=xueshu_se.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
6.  Demoriane H  Funambulus / funambule: Rope walkers & equilibrists: A potted history using quotes & anecdotes through the centuries. London: The Blondin Memorial Trust, 1997. [cited 20 March 2021]. Available from: http://www.blondinmemorialtrust.com/funambulus.html.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
7.  Lucas C, Galway-Witham J, Stringer CB, Bello SM. Investigating the use of paleolithic perforated batons: new evidence from Gough’s cave (Somerset, UK). Archaeol Anthropol Sci. 2019;11:5231-5255.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
8.  Diamond JM  Guns, germs and steel: A short history of everybody for the last 13,000 years. New York: Random House, 1998: 480.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
9.  Tayler J  National Geographic 2018. [cited 20 March 2021]. Available from: https://www.nationalgeographic.com/contests/article/photo-contest-2018-winners.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
10.  Tipton CM. The history of "Exercise Is Medicine" in ancient civilizations. Adv Physiol Educ. 2014;38:109-117.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 94]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 76]  [Article Influence: 7.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
11.  Badisches Landesmuseum Karlsruhe  Germany: Getty Images, 3000-1700 BC. [cited 20 March 2021]. Available from: https://www.landesmuseum.de/en/museum/museum-in-the-palace/permanent-exhibitions/ancient-cultures.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
12.  Bickerman EJ  Chronology of the ancient world (aspects of Greek and Roman life). 2nd ed. Ithaca, New York: Cornell University, 1982: 75.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
13.  Museo Archeologico Nazionale N  Sileni tightrope walkers, fresco from Villa of Cicero at Pompeii, Campania, Italy. Naples, (Archaeological Museum): Getty Images, 100-50 BC. [cited 20 March 2021]. Available from: https://www.museoarcheologiconapoli.it/en/room-and-sections-of-the-exhibition/frescoes/.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
14.  Scarborough J  Galen and the gladiators. In: Episteme: Revista critica di storia delle scienze mediche ebiologiche, 2013: 98-111.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
15.  Rom K  The slackline for the discipline of exercise and sports, 2009. [cited 20 March 2021]. Available from: https://www.bisp-surf.de/Record/PU201101000486.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
16.  Volery S  The slackline as a training device for snow sports: A trend sport with potential for coordination training in snow sports? 2009. [cited 20 March 2021]. Available from: https://www.bisp-surf.de/Record/PU201010007393.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
17.  Mahaffey BJ  The physiological effects of slacklining on balance and core strength. La Crosse: University of Wisconsin, 2009. [cited 20 March 2021]. Available from: https://minds.wisconsin.edu/handle/1793/37462.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
18.  de Franceschi PA  Le slackline, un outil prophylactique au service des joueurs de badminton de haut niveau. Val de Marne France, 2012.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
19.  Berryman JW. Motion and rest: Galen on exercise and health. Lancet. 2012;380:210-211.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 20]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 14]  [Article Influence: 1.2]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
20.  McClellan JE, Dorn H.   Science and technology in world history: An introduction. 3rd ed. Baltimore, Maryland: USA John Hopkins University (JHU) Press, 2015.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
21.  Galen C  Galeni opera omnia. Kühn KG, Editor and Translator. Leipzig, 1821-1823.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
22.  Galen C  A translation of Galen’s hygiene (de sanitate tuenda). Illinois: Charles C Thomas Publisher, 1951: 277.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
23.  Galen C  Galeni de curandi ratione per sanguinis missionem liber (Latin - Galen on the treatment of blood-letting) Theodorico Gaudano (Interprete-Translater). Paris: Christianus Vechel, 1529: 88.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
24.  Shoja MM, Tubbs RS, Ghabili K, Griessenauer CJ, Balch MW, Cuceu M. The Roman Empire legacy of Galen (129-200 AD). Childs Nerv Syst. 2015;31:1-5.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 9]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 9]  [Article Influence: 1.0]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
25.  Hippocrates A, Galen C, Translated by Coxe JR.   The writings of Hippocrates and Galen. In: Coxe JR. Epitomised from the Original Latin translations. Philadelphia: Lindsay and Blakiston, 1846.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
26.  Galen C  To Thrasyboulos: Is healthiness a part of medicine or of gymnastics? Originally written in 2nd Century AD. New York: Oxford University, 1997.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
27.  Gabel CP, Mokhtarinia HR, Melloh M, Mateo S. Slacklining as therapy to address non-specific low back pain in the presence of multifidus arthrogenic muscle inhibition. World J Orthop. 2021;12:178-196.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in CrossRef: 3]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 2]  [Article Influence: 0.7]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
28.  Rice DA, McNair PJ, Lewis GN, Dalbeth N. Quadriceps arthrogenic muscle inhibition: the effects of experimental knee joint effusion on motor cortex excitability. Arthritis Res Ther. 2014;16:502.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 50]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 62]  [Article Influence: 6.2]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
29.  Sonnery-Cottet B, Saithna A, Quelard B, Daggett M, Borade A, Ouanezar H, Thaunat M, Blakeney WG. Arthrogenic muscle inhibition after ACL reconstruction: a scoping review of the efficacy of interventions. Br J Sports Med. 2019;53:289-298.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 59]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 88]  [Article Influence: 14.7]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
30.  Vulpian A  Leçons sur l'appareil vaso-moteur (physiologie et pathologie). Faites à la Faculté de Médecine de Paris (French). Paris: Libraire Gerner-Baillière Et Ce, 1875.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
31.  Harding AEB. Arthritic muscular atrophy. J Path and Bact. 1925;179-187.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 7]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 7]  [Article Influence: 0.1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
32.  Stokes M, Young A. The contribution of reflex inhibition to arthrogenous muscle weakness. Clin Sci (Lond). 1984;67:7-14.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 257]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 226]  [Article Influence: 5.7]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
33.  Rice DA, McNair PJ. Quadriceps arthrogenic muscle inhibition: neural mechanisms and treatment perspectives. Semin Arthritis Rheum. 2010;40:250-266.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 261]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 303]  [Article Influence: 20.2]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
34.  Freeman S, Mascia A, McGill S. Arthrogenic neuromusculature inhibition: a foundational investigation of existence in the hip joint. Clin Biomech (Bristol, Avon). 2013;28:171-177.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 42]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 42]  [Article Influence: 3.8]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
35.  BLOCKEY NJ. An observation concerning the flexor muscles during recovery of function after dislocation of the elbow. J Bone Joint Surg Am. 1954;36-A:833-840.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
36.  Palmieri RM, Ingersoll CD, Hoffman MA, Cordova ML, Porter DA, Edwards JE, Babington JP, Krause BA, Stone MB. Arthrogenic muscle response to a simulated ankle joint effusion. Br J Sports Med. 2004;38:26-30.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 53]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 56]  [Article Influence: 2.8]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
37.  Russo M, Deckers K, Eldabe S, Kiesel K, Gilligan C, Vieceli J, Crosby P. Muscle Control and Non-specific Chronic Low Back Pain. Neuromodulation. 2018;21:1-9.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 72]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 106]  [Article Influence: 15.1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
38.  Cavaignac E, Daguzan C.   Improvement of knowledge about the arthrogenic muscle inhibition in the aftermath of knee trauma. [accessed 20 March 2021]. In: ClinicalTrials.gov [Internet]. Bethesda (MD): U.S. National Library of Medicine. Available from: http://clinicaltrials.gov/show/NCT03950024 ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT03950024.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
39.  Gabel CP, Rando N, Melloh M. Slacklining and stroke: A rehabilitation case study considering balance and lower limb weakness. World J Orthop. 2016;7:513-518.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in CrossRef: 5]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 4]  [Article Influence: 0.5]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
40.  Santos L, Fernandez-Rio J, Winge K, Barragán-Pérez B, Rodríguez-Pérez V, González-Díez V, Blanco-Traba M, Suman OE, Philip Gabel C, Rodríguez-Gómez J. Effects of supervised slackline training on postural instability, freezing of gait, and falls efficacy in people with Parkinson's disease. Disabil Rehabil. 2017;39:1573-1580.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 22]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 21]  [Article Influence: 2.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
41.  Pfusterschmied J, Buchecker M, Keller M, Wagner H, Taube W, Müller E. Supervised slackline training improves postural stability. Eur J Sports Sc. 2013;13:49-57.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 26]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 17]  [Article Influence: 1.5]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
42.  Santos L, Garcia BF, Fernandez-Rio J, Jakobsen MD. The effects of supervised slackline training on postural balance in judoists. Med Sport. 2014;67:539-553.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
43.  Donath L, Roth R, Zahner L, Faude O. Slackline Training (Balancing Over Narrow Nylon Ribbons) and Balance Performance: A Meta-Analytical Review. Sports Med. 2017;47:1075-1086.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 36]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 30]  [Article Influence: 5.0]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
44.  Ringhof S, Zeeb N, Altmann S, Neumann R, Woll A, Stein T. Short-term slackline training improves task-specific but not general balance in female handball players. Eur J Sport Sci. 2019;19:557-566.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 8]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 8]  [Article Influence: 1.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
45.  Jäger T, Kiefer J, Werner I, Federolf PA. Could Slackline Training Complement the FIFA 11+ Programme Regarding Training of Neuromuscular Control? Eur J Sport Sci. 2017;17:1021-1028.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 10]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 10]  [Article Influence: 1.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
46.  Jensen J, Ebben W, Petushek E, Richter C.   A case study on balance recovery in slacklining. In: Proceedings of the ISBS-28th Int Conference on Biomechanics in Sports, 2018.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
47.  Peterka RJ. Sensory integration for human balance control. Handb Clin Neurol. 2018;159:27-42.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 65]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 123]  [Article Influence: 24.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
48.  Volery S, Singh N, de Bruin ED, List R, Jaeggi MM, Mattli Baur B, Lorenzetti S. Traditional balance and slackline training are associated with task-specific adaptations as assessed with sensorimotor tests. Eur J Sport Sci. 2017;17:838-846.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 12]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 12]  [Article Influence: 1.7]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
49.  Davies GJ, Ellenbecker T.   Application of isokinetics in testing and rehabilitation. In: Andrews J, Harrelson GL and Wilk K. Physical rehabilitation of the injured athlete, 4th ed. Philadelphia, Saunders, 2004: 216-242.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
50.  MacKinnon CD, Winter DA. Control of whole body balance in the frontal plane during human walking. J Biomech. 1993;26:633-644.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 516]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 455]  [Article Influence: 14.7]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
51.  Keller M, Pfusterschmied J, Buchecker M, Müller E, Taube W. Improved postural control after slackline training is accompanied by reduced H-reflexes. Scand J Med Sci Sports. 2012;22:471-477.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 76]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 74]  [Article Influence: 5.7]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
52.  Granacher U, Muehlbauer T, Gollhofer A, Kressig RW, Zahner L. An intergenerational approach in the promotion of balance and strength for fall prevention - a mini-review. Gerontology. 2011;57:304-315.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 83]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 91]  [Article Influence: 6.5]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
53.  Vallery H, Neumann J.   Balancing on slacklines: Modeling and empirical evaluation, 2013. [cited 20 March 2021]. Available from: http://www.cmu.edu/dynamic-walking/files/abstracts/Vallery_2013_DW.pdf.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
54.  Mullen K, Ennis DM. Mathematical formulation of multivariate euclidean models for discrimination methods. Psychometrika. 1987;52:235-249.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
55.  Scholz JP, Schöner G. The uncontrolled manifold concept: identifying control variables for a functional task. Exp Brain Res. 1999;126:289-306.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 973]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 914]  [Article Influence: 36.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
56.  Maurer C, Peterka RJ. A new interpretation of spontaneous sway measures based on a simple model of human postural control. J Neurophysiol. 2005;93:189-200.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 305]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 274]  [Article Influence: 13.7]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
57.  Asai Y, Tasaka Y, Nomura K, Nomura T, Casadio M, Morasso P. A model of postural control in quiet standing: robust compensation of delay-induced instability using intermittent activation of feedback control. PLoS One. 2009;4:e6169.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 163]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 141]  [Article Influence: 9.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
58.  Milton J, Townsend JL, King MA, Ohira T. Balancing with positive feedback: the case for discontinuous control. Philos Trans A Math Phys Eng Sci. 2009;367:1181-1193.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 64]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 30]  [Article Influence: 2.0]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
59.  Halakatti SCP, Kengangutti A, Baddi S. Generating a measure manifold. Int J Math Arch. 2015;6.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
60.  Murray RM, Zexiang L, Sastry SS.   A mathematical introduction to robotic manipulation. London: CRC Press, 1994.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
61.  Schöner G. Recent developments and problems in human movement science and their conceptual implications. Ecol Psych. 1995;7:291-314.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 160]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 161]  [Article Influence: 5.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
62.  Delignières D, Torre K, Bernard PL. Transition from persistent to anti-persistent correlations in postural sway indicates velocity-based control. PLoS Comput Biol. 2011;7: e1001089.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 84]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 88]  [Article Influence: 6.8]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
63.  Nishikawa K, Biewener AA, Aerts P, Ahn AN, Chiel HJ, Daley MA, Daniel TL, Full RJ, Hale ME, Hedrick TL, Lappin AK, Nichols TR, Quinn RD, Satterlie RA, Szymik B. Neuromechanics: an integrative approach for understanding motor control. Integr Comp Biol. 2007;47:16-54.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 167]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 161]  [Article Influence: 9.5]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
64.  Guy B  Remarks on what is called "time perception" in psychology and neurophysiology. HAL, French CNRS open archives 2019. [cited 20 March 2021]. Available from: https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-02195919.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
65.  Qu X, Nussbaum MA, Madigan ML. A balance control model of quiet upright stance based on an optimal control strategy. J Biomech. 2007;40:3590-3597.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 34]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 35]  [Article Influence: 2.1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
66.  Stepan G. Delay effects in the human sensory system during balancing. Philos Trans A Math Phys Eng Sci. 2009;367:1195-1212.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 116]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 50]  [Article Influence: 3.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
67.  Serrien B, Hohenauer E, Clijsen R, Baeyens JP, Küng U.   Balance coordination strategies on slacklines: Analysis by means of self-organizing maps. In: 5th Motor Control Conference: Bridging the gap between biomechanics and motor control - Wisla, Poland, 2016. [cited 20 March 2021]. Available from: https://researchportal.vub.be/nl/publications/balance-coordination-strategies-on-slacklines-analysis-by-means-o.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
68.  Serrien B, Hohenauer E, Clijsen R, Taube W, Baeyens JP, Küng U. Changes in balance coordination and transfer to an unlearned balance task after slackline training: a self-organizing map analysis. Exp Brain Res. 2017;235:3427-3436.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 17]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 20]  [Article Influence: 2.9]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
69.  Kodama K, Kikuchi Y, Yamagiwa H.   Relation between bimanual coordination and whole-body balancing on a slackline. In: Steadman JR. CogSci - Annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 2016. [cited 20 March 2021]. Available from: https://xueshu.baidu.com/usercenter/paper/show?paperid=30651f386470dce2b292ca36d3291e2e&site=xueshu_se.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
70.  Peterka RJ. Sensorimotor integration in human postural control. J Neurophysiol. 2002;88:1097-1118.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 1299]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 1273]  [Article Influence: 57.9]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
71.  Stepan G, Kollar L. Balancing with reflex delay. Math Comp Model. 2000;31.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 42]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 27]  [Article Influence: 1.1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
72.  Peterka RJ. Postural control model interpretation of stabilogram diffusion analysis. Biol Cybern. 2000;82:335-343.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 278]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 227]  [Article Influence: 9.5]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
73.  Engel GL. The need for a new medical model: a challenge for biomedicine. Science. 1977;196:129-136.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 6292]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 4940]  [Article Influence: 105.1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
74.  McMahon AP  Leonardo da Vinci 1452-1519, treatise on painting (Codex Urbinas Latinus 1270) translated and annotated. Princeton, USA: Princeton University Press, 1956.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
75.  Donders FC  Contribution to the study of the movements of the human eye. In: Van Deen J, Donders FC and Moleschott J. Dutch contributions to the anatomical and physiological sciences. Dusseldorf und Utrecht: Butticher'sche Buchhandlung, 1848: 104-145.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
76.  Listing JB  Listing's gesetz (Listing’s law). In: Ruete CG. Lehrbuch der ophthalmologie fur aerzte and studierende (German) Textbook of ophthalmology for physicians and students. Braunschweig, Germany: Friedrich Vieweg & Sohn, 1853.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
77.  Listing JB  Handbuch der physiologischen optik (German). Handbook of physiological optics. Hamburg: Voss, 1866.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
78.  von Helmholtz HLF  Handbuch der physiologischen optik (German). Handbook of physiological optics. Hamburg: Voss, 1866.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
79.  Fick A. Die bewegungen des menschlichen augapfelf (German). Z Rationelle Medizin. 1854;4:101-128.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
80.  von Helmholtz HLF  Preliminary report on the reproductive rate of nerve conduction. Berlin: Veit & Comp, 1850: 71-73.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
81.  Donders FC  On the speed of mental processes, 2002.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 850]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 746]  [Article Influence: 13.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
82.  Merkel J. Die zeithchen verhaltnisse der. Willensthatigkeit (German) the temporal relationships of willed activity. PMos St. 1885;2:73-127.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
83.  Hick WE. On the rate of gain of information. J Exp Psychol. 1952;4:11-26.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 1423]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 803]  [Article Influence: 133.8]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
84.  Williamson JM, Lyons DA. Myelin Dynamics Throughout Life: An Ever-Changing Landscape? Front Cell Neurosci. 2018;12:424.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 74]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 100]  [Article Influence: 16.7]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
85.  Almeida RG, Lyons DA. On Myelinated Axon Plasticity and Neuronal Circuit Formation and Function. J Neurosci. 2017;37:10023-10034.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 124]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 138]  [Article Influence: 19.7]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
86.  Nijhawan R. Visual prediction: psychophysics and neurophysiology of compensation for time delays. Behav Brain Sci. 2008;31:179-98; discussion 198.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 126]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 135]  [Article Influence: 8.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
87.  Fraisse P. Perception and estimation of time. Annu Rev Psychol. 1984;35:1-36.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 559]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 415]  [Article Influence: 10.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
88.  Fitts PM. The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. 1954. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1992;121:262-269.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 145]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 82]  [Article Influence: 2.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
89.  Horak FB. Postural orientation and equilibrium: what do we need to know about neural control of balance to prevent falls? Age Ageing. 2006;35 Suppl 2:ii7-ii11.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 1188]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 1285]  [Article Influence: 71.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
90.  Graham-Brown T. Note on some Dynamic Principles Involved In Progression. BMJ. 1912;2:785-787.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
91.  Bresler B, Frankel J. The forces and moments in the leg during level walking. Trans ASME. 1950;72:27-36.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
92.  Saunders JB, Inman VT, Eberhart HD. The major determinants in normal and pathological gait. J Bone Joint Surg Am. 1953;35-A:543-558.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
93.  Adams JA. A closed-loop theory of motor learning. J Mot Behav. 1971;3:111-149.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 986]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 713]  [Article Influence: 37.5]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
94.  Loram ID, Lakie M. Direct measurement of human ankle stiffness during quiet standing: the intrinsic mechanical stiffness is insufficient for stability. J Physiol. 2002;545:1041-1053.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 317]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 255]  [Article Influence: 11.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
95.  Butera KA, Fox EJ, George SZ. Toward a Transformed Understanding: From Pain and Movement to Pain With Movement. Phys Ther. 2016;96:1503-1507.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 38]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 55]  [Article Influence: 6.9]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
96.  Küng UM, Horlings CG, Honegger F, Duysens JE, Allum JH. Control of roll and pitch motion during multi-directional balance perturbations. Exp Brain Res. 2009;194:631-645.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 15]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 11]  [Article Influence: 0.7]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
97.  Taube W, Gruber M, Gollhofer A. Spinal and supraspinal adaptations associated with balance training and their functional relevance. Acta Physiol (Oxf). 2008;193:101-116.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 204]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 220]  [Article Influence: 13.8]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
98.  Singh RE, White G, Delis I, Iqbal K. Alteration of muscle synergy structure while walking under increased postural constraints. Cog Comput Sys. 2020;50-56.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 8]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 9]  [Article Influence: 2.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
99.  Taubert M, Mehnert J, Pleger B, Villringer A. Rapid and specific gray matter changes in M1 induced by balance training. Neuroimage. 2016;133:399-407.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 54]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 66]  [Article Influence: 8.3]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
100.  Hüfner K, Binetti C, Hamilton DA, Stephan T, Flanagin VL, Linn J, Labudda K, Markowitsch H, Glasauer S, Jahn K, Strupp M, Brandt T. Structural and functional plasticity of the hippocampal formation in professional dancers and slackliners. Hippocampus. 2011;21:855-865.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 24]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 49]  [Article Influence: 3.5]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
101.  Handzel A, Flash T. Geometric methods in the study of human motor control. Cognitive Studies. 1999;6:309-321.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
102.  Flash T, Meirovitch Y, Barliya A. Models of human movement: Trajectory planning and inverse kinematics studies. Robot Auton Syst. 2013;61:330-339.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]
103.  Kang SB, Ikeuchi K. Toward automatic robot instruction from perception-temporal segmentation of tasks from human hand motion. IEEE Trans Robot Autom. 1995;11:670-681.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 62]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 47]  [Article Influence: 1.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
104.  Flash T, Hogan N. The coordination of arm movements: an experimentally confirmed mathematical model. J Neurosci. 1985;5:1688-1703.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 2749]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 1692]  [Article Influence: 43.4]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
105.  Donath L, Roth R, Zahner L, Faude O. Slackline training and neuromuscular performance in seniors: A randomized controlled trial. Scand J Med Sci Sports. 2016;26:275-283.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 34]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 36]  [Article Influence: 4.0]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
106.  Kodama K, Kikuchi Y, Yamagiwa H. Whole-body coordination skill for dynamic balancing on a slackline. LNCS. 2017;.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 4]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 1]  [Article Influence: 0.1]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
107.  Baurès R, Benguigui N, Amorim MA, Siegler IA. Intercepting free falling objects: better use Occam's razor than internalize Newton's law. Vision Res. 2007;47:2982-2991.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 25]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 27]  [Article Influence: 1.6]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
108.  McIntyre J, Zago M, Berthoz A, Lacquaniti F. Does the brain model Newton's laws? Nat Neurosci. 2001;4:693-694.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 248]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 241]  [Article Influence: 10.5]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]
109.  Hodges PW. Changes in motor planning of feedforward postural responses of the trunk muscles in low back pain. Exp Brain Res. 2001;141:261-266.  [PubMed]  [DOI]  [Cited in This Article: ]  [Cited by in Crossref: 253]  [Cited by in F6Publishing: 257]  [Article Influence: 11.2]  [Reference Citation Analysis (0)]